Read Ebook: Encyclopaedia Britannica 11th Edition Arculf to Armour Philip Volume 2 Slice 5 by Various
Font size:
Background color:
Text color:
Add to tbrJar First Page Next Page
Ebook has 271 lines and 86413 words, and 6 pages
may even be used in combination; thus 16 may be followed by 16a and 16b, and these by 17, and in such a case the ordinal 100 does not correspond with the total number up to this point.
Arithmetic is supposed to deal with cardinal, not with ordinal numbers; but it will be found that actual numeration, beyond about three or four, is based on the ordinal aspect of number, and that a scientific treatment of the subject usually requires a return to this fundamental basis.
In using an ordinal we direct our attention to a term of a series, while in using a cardinal we direct our attention to the interval between two terms. The total number in the series is the sum of the two cardinal numbers obtained by counting up to any interval from the beginning and from the end respectively; but if we take the ordinal numbers from the beginning and from the end we count one term twice over. Hence, if there are 365 days in a year, the 100th day from the beginning is the 266th, not the 265th, from the end.
Underlying this definition is a certain assumption, viz. that if we take the objects in a different order, the last symbol attached will still be 253. This, in an elementary treatment of the subject, must be regarded as axiomatic; but it is really a simple case of mathematical induction. If we take two objects A and B, it is obvious that whether we take them as A, B, or as B, A, we shall in each case get the sequence 1, 2. Suppose this were true for, say, eight objects, marked 1 to 8. Then, if we introduce another object anywhere in the series, all those coming after it will be displaced so that each will have the mark formerly attached to the next following; and the last will therefore be 9 instead of 8. This is true, whatever the arrangement of the original objects may be, and wherever the new one is introduced; and therefore, if the theorem is true for 8, it is true for 9. But it is true for 2; therefore it is true for 3; therefore for 4, and so on.
A brief account of the development of the system will be found under NUMERAL. Here we are concerned with the principle, the explanation of which is different according as we proceed on the grouping or the counting system.
On the grouping system we may in the first instance consider that we have separate symbols for numbers from "one" to "nine," but that when we reach ten objects we put them in a group and denote this group by the symbol used for "one," but printed in a different type or written of a different size or of a different colour. Similarly when we get to ten tens we denote them by a new representation of the figure denoting one. Thus we may have:
ones 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 tens 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 hundreds, 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 &c. / &c. &c.
On the counting system we may consider that we have a series of objects , and that we attach to these objects in succession the symbols 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 0, repeating this series indefinitely. There is as yet no distinction between the first object marked 1 and the second object marked 1. We can, however, attach to the 0's the same symbols, 1, 2, ... 0 in succession, in a separate column, repeating the series indefinitely; then do the same with every 0 of this new series; and so on. Any particular object is then defined completely by the combination of the symbols last written down in each series; and this combination of symbols can equally be used to denote the number of objects up to and including the last one .
In writing down a number in excess of 1000 it is usual in England and America to group the figures in sets of three, starting from the right, and to mark off the sets by commas. On the continent of Europe the figures are taken in sets of three, but are merely spaced, the comma being used at the end of a number to denote the commencement of a decimal.
The zero, called "nought," is of course a different thing from the letter O of the alphabet, but there may be a historical connexion between them . It is perhaps interesting to note that the latter-day telephone operator calls 1907 "nineteen O seven" instead of "nineteen nought seven."
If the numbers were written down in succession, they would naturally proceed from left to right, thus:--1, 2, 3,... This system, however, would require that in passing to "double figures" the figure denoting tens should be written either above or below the figure denoting ones, e.g.
The placing of the tens-figure to the left of the ones-figure will not seem natural unless the number-series runs either up or down.
In writing down any particular number, the successive powers of ten are written from right to left, e.g. 5,462,198 is
the small figures in brackets indicating the successive powers. On the other hand, in writing decimals, the sequence is from left to right.
In making out lists, schedules, mathematical tables , statistical tables, &c., the numbers are written vertically downwards. In the case of lists and schedules the numbers are only ordinals; but in the case of mathematical or statistical tables they are usually regarded as cardinals, though, when they represent values of a continuous quantity, they must be regarded as ordinals .
In graphic representation measurements are usually made upwards; the adoption of this direction resting on certain deeply rooted ideas .
This question of direction is of importance in reference to the development of useful number-forms ; and the existence of the two methods mentioned under and above produces confusion in comparing numerical tabulation with graphical representation. It is generally accepted that the horizontal direction of increase, where a horizontal direction is necessary, should be from left to right; but uniformity as regards vertical direction could only be attained either by printing mathematical tables upwards or by taking "downwards," instead of "upwards," as the "positive" direction for graphical purposes. The downwards direction will be taken in this article as the normal one for succession of numbers , and, where the arrangement is horizontal, it is to be understood that this is for convenience of printing. It should be noticed that, in writing the components of a number 253 as 200, 50 and 3, each component beneath the next larger one, we are really adopting the downwards principle, since the figures which make up 253 will on this principle be successively 2, 5 and 3 ).
The system is therefore essentially a cardinal and grouping one, i.e. it represents a number as the sum of sets of other numbers. It is therefore remarkable that it should now only be used for ordinal purposes, while the Hindu system, which is ordinal in its nature, since a single series is constantly repeated, is used almost exclusively for cardinal numbers. This fact seems to illustrate the truth that the counting principle is the fundamental one, to which the interpretation of grouped numbers must ultimately be referred.
The diversity of scales appears to be due mainly to four causes: the tendency to group into scores ; the tendency to subdivide into twelve; the tendency to subdivide into two or four, with repetitions, making subdivision into sixteen or sixty-four; and the independent adoption of different units for measuring the same kind of magnitude.
Where there is a division into sixteen parts, a binary scale may be formed by dividing into groups of two, four or eight. Thus the weights ordinarily in use for measuring from 1/4 oz. up to 2 lb. give the basis for a binary scale up to not more than eight figures, only 0 and 1 being used. The points of the compass might similarly be expressed by numbers in a binary scale; but the numbers would be ordinal, and the expressions would be analogous to those of decimals rather than to those of whole numbers.
In order to apply arithmetical processes to a quantity expressed in two or more denominations, we must first express it in terms of a single denomination by means of a varying scale of notation. Thus ?254, 13S. 6d. may be written
?254 " 13s. " 6d.;
each of the numbers in brackets indicating the number of units in one denomination that go to form a unit in the next higher denomination. To express the quantity in terms of ?, it ought to be written
?254 " 13 " 6;
this would mean ?254 13/20 or ?), and therefore would involve a fractional number.
Two exceptions, however, may be noted.
The number ten having been taken as the basis of numeration, there are various methods that might consistently be adopted for naming large numbers.
Names might be given to the successive powers of ten, up to the point to which numeration of ones is likely to go. Partial applications of this method are found in many languages.
A compromise between the last two methods would be to have names for the series of numbers, beginning with ten, each of which is the "square" of the preceding one. This would in effect be analysing numbers into components of the form a. 10^b where a is less than 10, and the index b is expressed in the binary scale, e.g. 7,000,000 would be 7 ? 10^4 ? 10?, and 700,000 would be 7 ? 10^4 ? 10^1.
The earliest Greek system of notation was similar to the Roman, except that the symbols for 50, 500, &c., were more complicated. Later, a system similar to the Hebrew was adopted, and extended by reproducing the first nine symbols of the series, preceded by accents, to denote multiplication by 1000.
On the island of Ceylon there still exists, or existed till recently, a system which combines some of the characteristics of the later Greek and the modern European notation; and it is conjectured that this was the original Hindu system.
For a further account of the above systems see NUMERAL, and the authorities quoted at the end of the present article.
On the other hand, it would seem that, for most educated people, sixteen and seventeen or twenty-six and twenty-seven, and even eighty-six and eighty-seven, are single numbers, just as six and seven are, and are not made up of groups of tens and ones. In other words, the denary scale, though adopted in notation and in numeration, does not arise in the corresponding mental concept until we get beyond 100.
Again, in the use of decimals, it is unusual to give less than two figures. Thus 3.142 or 3.14 would be quite intelligible; but 3.1 does not convey such a good idea to most people as either 3 or 3.10, i.e. as an expression denoting a fraction or a percentage.
There appears therefore to be a tendency to use some larger number than ten as a basis for grouping into new units or for subdivision into parts. The Babylonians adopted 60 for both these purposes, thus giving us the sexagesimal division of angles and of time.
This view is supported, not only by the intelligibility of percentages to ordinary persons, but also by the tendency, noted above , to group years into centuries, and to avoid the use of thousands. Thus 1876 is not 1 thousand, 8 hundred, 7 tens and 6, but 18 hundred and 76, each of the numbers 18 and 76 being named as if it were a single number. It is also in accordance with what is so far known about number-forms .
In the case of young children the limit is probably two. That this was also the limit in the case of primitive races, and that the classification of things was into one, two and many, before any definite process of counting came to be adopted, is clear from the use of the "dual number" in language, and from the way in which the names for three and four are often based on those for one and two. With the individual, as with the race, the limit of the number that can be seen gradually increases up to four or five.
The statement that a number of objects can be seen to be three or four is not to be taken as implying that there is a simultaneous perception of all the objects. The attention may be directed in succession to the different objects, so that the perception is rhythmical; the distinctive rhythm thus aiding the perception of the particular number.
In consequence of this limitation of the power of perception of number, it is practically impossible to use a pure denary scale in elementary number-teaching. If a quinary-binary system is not adopted, teachers unconsciously resort to a binary-quinary system. This is commonly done where cubes are used; thus seven is represented by three pairs of cubes, with a single cube at the top.
The forms are varied, and have few points in common; but the following tendencies are indicated.
In the majority of cases the numbers lie on a continuous line.
There is nearly always a break in direction at 12. From 1 to 12 the numbers sometimes lie in the circumference of a circle, an arrangement obviously suggested by a clock-face; in these cases the series usually mounts upwards from 12. In a large number of cases, however, the direction is steadily upwards from 1 to 12, then changing. In some cases the initial direction is from right to left or from left to right; but there are very few in which it is downwards.
The multiples of 10 are usually strongly marked; but special stress is also laid on other important numbers, e.g. the multiples of 12.
The series sometimes goes up to very high numbers, but sometimes stops at 100, or even earlier. It is not stated, in most cases, whether all the numbers within the limits of the series have definite positions, or whether there are only certain numbers which form an essential part of the figure, while others only exist potentially. Probably the latter is almost universally the case.
Amongst some of the lowest tribes, as amongst animals, the only differentiation is between one and many, or between one, two and many, or between one, two, three and many. As it becomes necessary to use higher but still small numbers, they are formed by combinations of one and two, or perhaps of three with one or two. Thus many of the Australasian and South American tribes use only one and two; seven, for instance, would be two two two one.
There are a few, but only a few, cases in which the number 6 or 8 is named as twice 3 or twice 4; and there are also a few cases in which 7, 8 and 9 are named as 6 + 1, 6 + 2 and 6 + 3. In the large majority of cases the numbers 6, 7, 8 and 9 are 5 + 1, 5 + 2, 5 + 3 and 5 + 4, being named either directly from their composition in this way or as the fingers on the second hand.
In a few cases the names of certain small numbers are the names of objects which present these numbers in some conspicuous way. Thus the word used by the Abipones to denote 5 was the name of a certain hide of five colours. It has been suggested that names of this kind may have been the origin of the numeral words of different races; but it is improbable that direct visual perception would lead to a name for a number unless a name based on a process of counting had previously been given to it.
When numbering begins, the names of the successive numbers are attached to the individual objects; thus the numbers are originally ordinal, not cardinal.
The development from the name-series to the quantitative conception is aided by the numbering of material objects and the performance of elementary processes of comparison, addition, &c., with them. It may also be aided, to a certain extent, by the tendency to find rhythms in sequences of sounds. This tendency is common in adults as well as in children; the strokes of a clock may, for instance, be grouped into fours, and thus eleven is represented as two fours and three. Finger-counting is of course natural to children, and leads to grouping into fives, and ultimately to an understanding of the denary system of notation.
FIG. 1.
FIG. 2. FIG. 3.
Add to tbrJar First Page Next Page